1. Petten C V, Kutas M. Ambiguous words in context: an event-related potential analysis of the time course of meaning activation.
Journal of Memory and Language, 1987, 26(2): 188 -208
2. Coulson S, Severens E. Hemispheric asymmetry and pun comprehension: when cowboys have sore calves. Brain and
Language, 2007, 100(2): 172 -187
3. Chang Y T, Ku L C, Wu C L, et al. Event-related potential (ERP ) evidence for the differential cognitive processing of
semantic jokes and pun jokes. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 2019, 31(2): 131 -144
4. Hempelmann C F, Miller T. Puns: taxonomy and phonology. Attardo S ( ed). The Routledge Handbook of Language and
Humor. New York, NY, USA: Routledge, 2017: 95 -108
5. Bischetti L, Ceccato I, Lecce S, et al. Pragmatics and theory of mind in older adults’ humor comprehension. Current Psychology,
2019, DOI: 10. 1007/ s12144-019-00295-w
6. Jared D, Bainbridge S. Reading homophone puns: evidence from eye tracking. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2017, 71(1): 2 -13
7. Rose S B, Spalek K, Rahman R A. Listening to puns elicits the co-activation of alternative homophone meanings during language
production. Plos One, 2017, 10(6): Article e0130853
8.Sheridan H, Reingold E M, Daneman M. Using puns to study contextual influences on lexical ambiguity resolution: evidence from
eye movements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2009, 16(5): 875 -881
9. Adamczyk P, Wyczesany M, Domagalik A, et al. Neural circuit of verbal humor comprehension in schizophrenia: an fMRI study.
NeuroImage: Clinical, 2017, 15: 525 -540
10. Kana R K, Wadsworth H M. “The archeologist’s career ended in ruins": hemispheric differences in pun comprehension in autism. Neuroimage, 2012, 62(1): 77 -86
11. Kutas M, Hillyard S A. Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 1980,
207(4427): 203 -205
12. Dholakia A, Meade G, Coch D. The N400 elicited by homonyms in puns: two primes are not better than one. Psychophysiology,
2016, 53(12): 1799 -1810
13. Coulson S, Williams R F. Hemispheric asymmetries and joke comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 2005, 19(3): 275 -288
14. Mayerhofer B, Schacht A. From incoherence to mirth: neuro- cognitive processing of garden-path jokes. Frontiers in Psychology, 2015, 6: Article 550
15.Kaan E, Harris A, Gibson E, et al. The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Language and Cognitive Processes,
2000, 15(2): 159 -201
16. Friederici A D. The time course of syntactic activation during language processing: a model based on neuropsychological and
neurophysiological data. Brain and Language, 1995, 50 (3 ): 259 -281
17. Delogu F, Brouwer H, Crocker M W. Event-related potentials index lexical retrieval ( N400) and integration ( P600) during
language comprehension. Brain and Cognition, 2019, 135: Article 103569
18. Huang Y L, Jiang M H, Guo Q, et al. Dissociation of the confounding influences of expectancy and integrative difficulty
residing in anomalous sentences in event-related potential studies. Journal of Visualized Experiments, 2019, DOI: 10. 3791/59436
19. Kolk H, Chwilla D. Late positivities in unusual situations. Brain and Language, 2007, 100(3): 257 -261
20. Canal P, Bischetti L, Di Paola S, et al. 'Honey, shall I change the baby?: well done, choose another one’: ERP and time-frequency correlates of humor processing. Brain and Cognition, 2019, 132: 41 -55
21. Goel V, Dolan R J. The functional anatomy of humor: segregating cognitive and affective components. Nature Neuroscience, 2001, 4(3): 237 -238
22. Samson A C, Zysset S, Huber O. Cognitive humor processing: different logical mechanisms in nonverbal cartoons: an fMRI study. Social Neuroscience, 2008, 3(2): 125 -140
23. Perfetti C A, Zhang S L. Very early phonological activation in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 1995, 21(1): 24 -33
24. Tan L H, Perfetti C A. Visual Chinese character recognition: does phonological information mediate access to meaning? Journal of Memory and Language, 1997, 37(1): 41 -57
25. Perfetti C A, Tan L H. The time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic activation in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 1998, 24(1): 101 -118
26. Ashby J, Sanders L D, Kingston J. Skilled readers begin processing sub-phonemic features by 80 ms during visual word
recognition: evidence from ERPs. Biological Psychology, 2009,80(1): 84 -94
27. Ashby J, Martin A E. Prosodic phonological representations early in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2008, 34(1): 224 -236
28. Ashby J. Phonology is fundamental in skilled reading: evidence from ERPs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2010, 17(1): 95 -
100
29. Hagoort P, Brown C, Groothusen J. The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language and
Cognitive Processes, 1993, 8(4): 439 -483
30. Gu V, Mohamed Ali O, L'Abbee Lacas K, et al. Investigating the effects of antipsychotics and schizotypy on the N400 using event-related potentials and semantic categorization. Journal of Visualized Experiments, 2014, DOI: 10. 3791/52082
31. Campbell J, Nielsen M, LaBrec A, et al. Stimulus-specific cortical visual evoked potential morphological patterns. Journal of
Visualized Experiments, 2019, DOI: 10. 3791/59146
32. Jin J, Li S R, Daly I, et al. The study of generic model set for reducing calibration time in P300-based brain-computer interface.
IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering, 2020, 28(1): 3 -12
33. Frisch S, Schlesewsky M, Saddy D, et al. The P600 as an indicator of syntactic ambiguity. Cognition, 2002, 85(3): B83 -
B92
34. Brouwer H, Fitz H, Hoeks J. Getting real about semantic illusions: Rethinking the functional role of the P600 in language
comprehension. Brain Research, 2012, 1446: 127 -143
35. Brouwer H, Crocker M W. On the proper treatment of the N400 and P600 in language comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology,
2017, DOI: 10. 3389/ fpsyg. 2017. 01327
|